Library

feed icon rss

Your email was sent successfully. Check your inbox.

An error occurred while sending the email. Please try again.

Proceed reservation?

Export
  • 1
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Springer
    Nutrient cycling in agroecosystems 37 (1994), S. 23-34 
    ISSN: 1573-0867
    Keywords: aluminium ; Freundlich equation ; iron ; Langmuir equation ; phosphorus-32 ; P-sorption capacity ; Temkin equation
    Source: Springer Online Journal Archives 1860-2000
    Topics: Agriculture, Forestry, Horticulture, Fishery, Domestic Science, Nutrition
    Notes: Abstract Total amounts of phosphate (P) sorbed were measured for 6 Portuguese soils of widely varying properties.32P was used to assess the isotopically exchangeable and non-exchangeable sorbed P. Total sorbed and exchangeable P were described by modified Freundlich equations and non-exchangeable P by a Temkin equation. The Langmuir equation also proved to fit the data for non-exchangeable P well. The amount of total sorbed P required to attain 0.2 mg P 1−1 in solution ranged from 5.3 to 819 mg P kg−1. At this concentration exchangeable and non-exchangeable P values varied from 62.4 to 536.6 and from 0.4 to 322.1 mg P kg−1 respectively. There were highly significant (p 〈 0.01) correlations between soil organic matter and all forms of sorbed P (total sorbed, exchangeable and non-exchangeable). The P sorption parameters with correlation coefficients greater than 0.967 were parametersa andb of the modified Freundlich equation bn of Temkin and parametersa of the Langmuir equation. Aluminium extracted by acid oxalate (Alox) and dithionite (Aldi) showed highly significant correlation coefficients (r = 0.972) with the same sorption parameters. But P sorption was not closely related to the clay content Feox and Fedi. It was concluded that extractable aluminium (Alox and Aldi) had the most important effects on P sorption in these soils.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Library Location Call Number Volume/Issue/Year Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 2
    ISSN: 1432-1106
    Keywords: Transformation ; Upper limb ; Vision ; Kinesthesia ; Human
    Source: Springer Online Journal Archives 1860-2000
    Topics: Medicine
    Notes: Abstract The abilities of human subjects to perform reach and grasp movements to remembered locations/ orientations of a cylindrical object were studied under four conditions: (1) visual presentation of the object — reach with vision allowed; (2) visual presentation — reach while blindfolded; (3) kinesthetic presentation of the object-reach while blindfolded and (4) kinesthetic presentation-reach with vision. The results showed that subjects were very accurate in locating the object in the purely kinesthetic condition and that directional errors were low in all four conditions; but, predictable errors in reach distance occurred in conditions 1,2, and 4. The pattern of these distance errors was similar to that identified in previous research using a pointing task to a small target (i.e., overshoots of close targets, undershoots of far targets). The observation that the pattern of distance errors in condition 4 was similar to that of conditions 1 and 2 suggests that subjects transform kinesthetically defined hand locations into a visual coordinate system when vision is available during upper limb motion to a remembered kinesthetic target. The differences in orientation of the upper limb between target and reach positions in condition 3 were similar in magnitude to the errors associated with kinesthetic perceptions of arm and hand orientations in three-dimensional space reported in previous studies. However, fingertip location was specified with greater accuracy than the orientation of upper limb segments. This was apparently accomplished by compensation of variations in shoulder (arm) angles with oppositely directed variations in elbow joint angles. Subjects were also able to transform visually perceived object orientation into an appropriate hand orientation for grasp, as indicated by the relation between hand roll angle and object orientation (elevation angle). The implications of these results for control of upper limb motion to external targets are discussed.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Library Location Call Number Volume/Issue/Year Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 3
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Springer
    Experimental brain research 112 (1996), S. 127-134 
    ISSN: 1432-1106
    Keywords: Coordinate system ; Transformation ; Human
    Source: Springer Online Journal Archives 1860-2000
    Topics: Medicine
    Notes: Abstract The purpose of the present experiment was to determine the preferred visual “straight ahead” or anterior/posterior (a/p) axis at the perceptual level. The ability of 12 neurologically normal, young adult subjects to position a rod parallel to the head and trunk a/p axes while viewing eccentrically located visual targets were studied under six conditions: 1. fixed — subjects stood erect with the head aligned to the trunk and viewed a central target while visually aligning a hand-held rod to the head and trunk a/p axis. 2. eyes — subjects moved only their eyes to view eccentric targets and aligned the rod to the head and trunk a/p axis. 3. head-trunk — subjects viewed the eccentric targets by rotating the head about a vertical axis and aligned the rod to the trunk a/p axis. 4. head-head — subjects viewed the targets as in 3 and positioned the rod parallel to the head a/p axis. 5. trunk-head — subjects viewed the targets by rotating the trunk and head as a unit about the vertical axis and aligned the rod parallel to the head a/p axis (note that the head and trunk a/p axes were misaligned by the experimenter prior to target viewing). 6. trunk-trunk — subjects viewed targets as in 5 and positioned the rod parallel to the trunk a/p axis. Subjects performed 25–35 consecutive trials within each condition. Perceptual errors were similar for aligning the rod to the trunk and head a/p axes; however, moving the trunk produced much larger constant and variable perceptual errors than moving the head. In a second experiment, four subjects controlled the position of a lighted rod held by a robot arm in complete darkness. They were instructed to align the rod to either the head or trunk a/p axis under conditions similar to the fixed, head-trunk, and head-head tasks described above. Perceptual errors were much larger when aligning the rod to the head a/p axis than to trunk a/p axis when the head was moved. This shows that the trunk a/p axis is clearly preferred at the perceptual level when visual background cues are not present. These data strongly suggest that the visual coordinate system uses a trunk-fixed a/p axis to define the subjective straight-ahead direction and right/left position of a target. Implications of these findings for sensorimotor transformations in control of upper limb movements to visual targets are discussed.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Library Location Call Number Volume/Issue/Year Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 4
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Springer
    Nutrient cycling in agroecosystems 34 (1993), S. 45-53 
    ISSN: 1573-0867
    Keywords: cation-anion-exchange resin ; fertilizer ; Olsen-P ; residual value ; phosphorus-32 ; sorghum
    Source: Springer Online Journal Archives 1860-2000
    Topics: Agriculture, Forestry, Horticulture, Fishery, Domestic Science, Nutrition
    Notes: Abstract The amounts of available P in a Typic Pellustert containing different levels of residual fertilizer P were assessed in a field experiment and chemically with cation-anion-exchange resin, using: (i) P desorbed after 1 extraction, (ii) cumulative P from 8 extractions [Resin8-P] and (iii) the asymptote of the extraction curve. P exchangeable to32P, Olsen-P and P extracted by dilute salt solution (0.03M KCl) were also measured. P available to sorghum in 1989 was strongly influenced by fertilizer P applied in 1988, but not by P applied in 1987. Olsen-P and Resin8-P both reflected well the effects of residual P on yield and P uptake and could therefore be used to predict available residual P. The amounts of Resin8-P were much larger than Olsen-P, so it was easier to observe the differences between soil treatments, but Olsen-P gave the better correlations with grain yield and P uptake. Multiple regressions of crop yield or P uptake with amounts of P applied in 1987 and 1988 gave the best correlations, without the need for practical work. Thus, the development of an appropriate model based on records of P fertilization is seen as the most effective way to predict availability of residual P. Assessments related to the P intensity and desorption rate using resin were not able to improve predictions of available P, indicating that within one soil the quantity of residual P is most important, but for comparisons between soils, account will need to be taken of differences in soil characteristics.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Library Location Call Number Volume/Issue/Year Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
Close ⊗
This website uses cookies and the analysis tool Matomo. More information can be found here...