Skip to main content
Log in

The role of the lateral suprasylvian visual cortex of the cat in object-background interactions: Permanent deficits following lesions

  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The contribution of the lateral suprasylvian cortex to pattern recognition was studied by behavioural detection experiments in combination with bilateral lesions of different parts of the lateral suprasylvian areas (LSA) and area 7 in seven cats. In a two-alternatives forced choice task the cats had to discriminate simple outline patterns which were additively superimposed on a structured visual background made up of broadband Gaussian noise. For various stimulus conditions (moving or stationary patterns and/or background) the detection probability (P D) of the cats was measured as a function of the signal to noise ratio (S/N). Each cat was tested before and after the lesion. Four different types of lesion could be distinguished depending on their extent: (1) lesion of parts of the (LSA); (2) lesion of parts of the LSA with undercutting of areas 17, 18 and 19; (3) lesion of area 7; (4) lesion of area 7 and parts of the LSA.

  1. 1.

    We found that a large bilateral lesion of the LSA led to significant deficits in all test situations which were dependent on the existence of relative velocity of moving patterns against a structured background. The ability of the cats to discriminate simple outline patterns which were kept stationary was not reduced. On the contrary, when they were tested with stationary and moving patterns on unfocused (empty) backgrounds, we found, to our great surprise, that the performance of the lesioned cats was significantly improved compared with intact animals. As these lesioned cats had no deficits with moving patterns on a uniformly grey background, we conclude that the deficits with the moving patterns must have been caused by interactions between patterns and background, and not by movement of a pattern per se.

  2. 2.

    As soon as the lesion of the LSA was extended by a bilateral undercutting of areas 17, 18 and 19 we found very severe deficits in all test situations, regardless of whether the patterns were moving or kept stationary, or whether they were superimposed on a background or not. The most substantial deficits occurred when the patterns were moving on a stationary background. In these situations the cats were no longer able to reach the 84% correct criterion. Again, the cats were able to reach criterion with moving patterns on a uniformly grey background indicating that this deficit is probably caused by the interaction of patterns and background and not by motion of the patterns per se.

  3. 3.

    A large lesion of area 7 led to modest but significant deficits of more or less the same degree in all test situations with the exception of quickly moving patterns on a structured background. In contrast, a much smaller lesion of area 7 yielded significant deficits only when the background was moved and there was a low relative velocity between the patterns and the background.

  4. 4.

    In brief, the combination of a lesion of area 7 with that of the LSA roughly provoked a combination of the effects of the two lesions (1 and 3) alone. We found significant deficits in all test situations.

We did not find evidence for any type of functional recovery in any of the lesions described. All deficits were permanent. Our results support the idea of a functional segregation between the LSA and area 7. They confirm that the LSA are involved in pattern recognition whenever it is associated with motion in combination with object-background interactions. This suggests an involvement of the LSA in the analysis of object- and self-induced motion. An interpretation of the results of lesioning area 7 is based on the conjecture that the mechanism of vergence movements or of binocular fusion of both retina images might be impaired. Under this condition it seems conceivable to expect double images or a reduced visual acuity because the images are out of focus. This would have a similar effect on the detection performance in each stimulus configuration. In addition, the results of lesion 4 provide evidence that both functional subunits (LSA and area 7) are not able to compensate for one another. Finally, we conclude from the results after lesion 2 that the LSA is part of a system which is based on the cooperation with striate cortex and requires intact primary cortex for its full function.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bando T (1985) Pupillary constriction evoked from the posterior medial lateral suprasylvian (PMLS) area in cats. Neurosci Res 2:472–485

    Google Scholar 

  • Bando T, Yamamoto N, Tsukuhara N (1984) Cortical neurons related to lens accommodation in posterior lateral suprasylvian area in cats. J Neurophysiol 52:879–891

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlucchi G, Sprague JM (1981) The cerebral cortex in visual learning and memory, and in interhemispheric transfer in the cat. In: Schmitt FO, Worden FG, Adelman G, Dennis SG (eds) The organization of the cerebral cortex. MIT, Cambridge, Mass., pp 415–440

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlucchi G, Sprague JM, Lepore F, Mascetti GG (1978) Effects of lesions of areas 17, 18 and 19 on interocular transfer of pattern discriminations in splitchiasm cats. Exp Brain Res 31:275–297

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlucchi G, Sprague JM, Antonini A, Simoni A (1979) Learning and interhemispheric transfer of visual pattern discriminations following unilateral suprasylvian lesions in splitchiasm cats. Exp Brain Res 34:551–574

    Google Scholar 

  • Camarda R, Rizzolatti G (1976) Visual receptive fields in the lateral suprasylvian area (Clare Bishop) of the cat. Brain Res 101:427–443

    Google Scholar 

  • Cornwell P, Overman W, Ross C (1980) Lesions of visual cortex impair discrimination of hidden figures by cats. Physiol Behav 24:533–540

    Google Scholar 

  • Dinse HRO, Krüger K (1987) Sensitivity of cat cortical neurons (areas 17, 18, 19, PMLS) to interaction between object and background local vs global processing revisited. Soc Neurosci Abstr 13:1624

    Google Scholar 

  • Dinse HRO, Krüger K (1990) Contribution of area 19 to the foreground background interaction of the cat: an analysis based on single cell recordings and behavioral experiments. Exp Brain Res 82:107–122

    Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson IML (1979) Responses in cat suprasylvian cortex (Clare Bishop Area) to stretch of extraocular muscles. J Physiol (Lond) 296:60

    Google Scholar 

  • Doty RW (1971) Survival of pattern vision after removal of striate cortex in the adult cat. J Comp Neurol 143:341–370

    Google Scholar 

  • Dow BM, Dubner R (1969) Visual receptive fields and responses to movement in an association area of cat cerebral cortex. J Neurophysiol 32:773–784

    Google Scholar 

  • Dow BM, Dubner R (1971) Single unit responses to moving stimuli in middle suprasylvian gyrus of the cat. J Neurophysiol 3:47–55

    Google Scholar 

  • Dubner R, Brown JF (1968) Responses of cells to restricted visual stimuli in an association area of cat cerebral cortex. Exp Neurol 20:70–86

    Google Scholar 

  • Fabre M, Buser P (1980) Structures involved in acquisition and performance of visually guided movements in the cat. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Warsz) 40:95–116

    Google Scholar 

  • Fabre M, Buser P (1981) Effects of lesioning the anterior suprasylvian cortex on visuo motor guidance performance in the cat. Exp Brain Res 41:81–88

    Google Scholar 

  • Frost BJ (1985) Neural mechanisms for detecting object motion and figure ground boundaries contrasted with selfmotion detecting systems. In: Ingle D, Jeannerod M, Lee D (eds) Brain mechanisms of spatial vision. Nijhoff, Dordrecht

    Google Scholar 

  • Grünau M von, Frost BJ (1983) Double opponent process mechanism underlying the RFstructure of directionally specific cells of cat's lateral suprasylvian visual area. Exp Brain Res 49:84–92

    Google Scholar 

  • Grünau M von, Zumbroich TJ, Poulin C (1987) Visual receptive field properties in the posterior suprasylvian cortex of the cat: a comparison between areas PMLS and PLLS. Vision Res 27:343–356

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamada T, Flandrin JM, Furuya N (1988) Latent defect in monocular optokinetic nystagmus after neonatal removal of the lateral suprasylvian area in the cat. Behav Brain Res 27:109–114

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardy SC, Stein BE (1988) Small lateral suprasylvian cortex lesions produce visual neglect and decreased visual activity in the superior colliculus. J Comp Neurol 273:527–542

    Google Scholar 

  • Hassler R, Muhs-Clement K (1964) Architektonischer Aufbau des sensomotorischen und parietalen Cortex der Katze. J Hirn-forsch 6:377–420

    Google Scholar 

  • Heath CJ, Jones EG (1971) The anatomical organization of the supraslvian gyrus of the cat. Ergeb Anat Entwicklungsgesch 45:3–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Herdman SJ, Tusa RJ, Smith CB (1989) Cortical areas involved in horizontal OKN in cats: metabolic activity. J Neurosci 9:1150–1162

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyvärinen J (1982) The parietal cortex of monkey and man. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Joseph JP, Giroud P (1986) Visuomotor properties of the anterior suprasylvian gyrus of the awake cat. Exp Brain Res 62:355–362

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy H, Magnin M (1977) Saccadic influences on single neuron activity in the medial bank of cats' suprasylvian sulcus (Clare Bishop area). Exp Brain Res 27:315–317

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiefer W, Krüger K, Strauss G, Berlucchi G (1989) Considerable deficits in the detection performance of the cat after lesion of the suprasylvian visual cortex. Exp Brain Res 75:208–212

    Google Scholar 

  • Komatsu Y, Shibuki K, Toyama K (1983) Eye movement related activities in cells of the lateral suprasylvian cortex of the cat. Neurosci Lett 41:271–276

    Google Scholar 

  • Krone G, Kunz D, von Seelen W (1983) On the analysis of the cat's pattern recognition system. Biol Cybern 48:115–124

    Google Scholar 

  • Krüger K, Dinse HRO (1988) Background dependence of direction selectivity in cortical neurons of areas 17, 18, 19, 7 and PMLS of the cat. Soc Neurosci Abstr 14:602

    Google Scholar 

  • Krüger K, Heitländer-Fansa H, Dinse HRO, Berlucchi G (1986) Detection performance of normal cats and those lacking areas 17 and 18: a behavioral approach to analyse pattern recognition deficits. Exp Brain Res 63:233–247

    Google Scholar 

  • Krüger K, Donicht M, Müller-Kusdian G, Kiefer W, Berlucchi G (1988) Lesion of areas 17/18/19: effects on the cat's performance in a binary detection task. Exp Brain Res 72:510–516

    Google Scholar 

  • Krüger K, Groh A, Kiefer W (1990) Lesion of area 7 and the lateral suprasylvian areas impair depth perception of cats (abstract). Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 31:399

    Google Scholar 

  • Krüger K, Kiefer W, Groh A (1993) Lesion of the suprasylvian cortex impairs depth perception of cats. Neuroreport 4:883–886

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen RJ, Marx ML (1986) An introduction to mathematical statistics and its applications. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawler KA, Cowey A (1986) The effects of pretectal and superior collicular lesions on binocular vision. Exp Brain Res 63:402–408

    Google Scholar 

  • Niimi K, Sprague JM (1970) Thalamocortical organization of the visual system in the cat. J Comp Neurol 138:219–250

    Google Scholar 

  • Otsuka R, Hassler R (1962) Über Aufbau und Gliederung der corticalen Sehsphäre bei der Katze. Arch Psychiatr Z Neurol 203:212–234

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer LA, Rosenquist AC, Tusa RJ (1978) The retinotopic organization of lateral suprasylvian visual areas in the cat. J Comp Neurol 177:237–256

    Google Scholar 

  • Pasternak T, Horn CM, Maunsell JHR (1989) Deficits in speed discrimination following lesions of the lateral suprasylvian cortex in the cat. Visual Neurosci 3:365–375

    Google Scholar 

  • Ptito M, Lepore F (1983) Effects of unilateral and bilateral lesions of the lateral suprasylvian area on learning and interhemispheric transfer of pattern discrimination in the cat. Behav Brain Res 7:211–227

    Google Scholar 

  • Rauschecker JP, Grünau MW von, Poulin C (1987a) Centrifugal organization of direction preference in the cat's lateral suprasylvian visual cortex. J Neurosci 7:943–958

    Google Scholar 

  • Rauschecker JP, Grünau MW von, Poulin C (1987b) Thalamocortical connections and their correlation with receptive field properties in the cat's lateral suprasylvian visual cortex. Exp Brain Res 67:100–112

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinoso-Suarez F (1961) Topographischer Hirnatlas der Katze. Merck, Darmstadt

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G, Camarda R (1975) Inhibition of visual responses of single units in the cat visual area of the lateral suprasylvian gyrus (Clare-Bishop area) by the introduction of a second visual stimulus. Brain Res 88:357–361

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G, Camarda R (1977) Influence of the presentation of remote visual stimuli of visual responses of cat area 17 and lateral suprasylvian area. Exp Brain Res 29:107–122

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenquist AC (1985) Connections of visual cortical areas in the cat. In: Peters A, Jones EG (eds) Visual Cortex. (Cerebral Cortex vol 3) Plenum, New York, pp 81–117

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson RT, Mayers KS, Teyler TJ, Bettinger LA, Birch H, Davis JL, Phillips DS, Thompson RF (1975) Unit activity in posterior association cortex of cat. J Neurophysiol 38:780–794

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson DA (1963) A method of measuring eye movement using a scleral search coil in a magnetic field. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 10:137–145

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanderson KJ (1971) The projection of the visual field to the lateral geniculate and medial interlaminar nuclei in the cat. J Comp Neurol 142:101–118

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith DC, Spear PD (1979) Effects of superior colliculus removal on receptive field properties of neurons in the lateral suprasylvian visual area of the cat. J Neurophysiol 42:57–75

    Google Scholar 

  • Spear PD (1991) Functions of extrastriate visual cortex in nonprimate species. In: Leventhal A (eds) The neural basis of visual function. (Vision and visual dysfunction, vol. 4) Macmillan, Basingstoke, UK, pp 339–370

    Google Scholar 

  • Spear PD, Baumann TP (1975) Receptive field characteristics of single neurons in lateral suprasylvian visual area of the cat. J Neurophysiol 38:1403–1420

    Google Scholar 

  • Spear PD, Miller S, Ohman L (1983) Effects of lateral suprasylvian visual cortex lesions on visual localisation, discrimination, and attention in cats. Behav Brain Res 10:339–359

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprague JM, Levy J, DiBerardino A, Berlucchi G (1977) Visual cortex areas mediating form discrimination in the cat. J Comp Neurol 172:441–488

    Google Scholar 

  • Straschill M, Schick F (1974) Neuronal activity during eye movements in a visual association area of cat cerebral cortex. Exp Brain Res 19:467–477

    Google Scholar 

  • Toyama K, Kozasa T (1982) Responses of Clare Bishop neurones to three dimensional movement of a light stimulus. Vision Res 22:571–574

    Google Scholar 

  • Toyama K, Komatsu Y, Kasai H, Fujii K, Umetani K (1985) Responsiveness of Clare Bishop neurons to visual cues associated with motion of a visual stimulus in three dimensional space. Vision Res 25:407–414

    Google Scholar 

  • Tusa RJ, Palmer LA, Rosenquist AC (1981) Multiple cortical visual areas: visual field topography in the cat. In: Woolsey CN (eds) Cortical sensory organization, vol 2. Humana, Clifton, NJ, 1–61

    Google Scholar 

  • Tusa RJ, Demer JL, Herdman SJ (1989) Cortical areas involved in OKN and VOR in cats: cortical lesions. J Neurosci 9:1163–1178

    Google Scholar 

  • Vanni-Mercier G, Magnin M (1982) Retinotopic organization of extra retinal saccade related input to the visual cortex in the cat. Exp Brain Res 46:368–376

    Google Scholar 

  • Ventre J (1985a) Cortical control of oculomotor functions. I. Optokinetic nystagmus. Behav Brain Res 15:211–226

    Google Scholar 

  • Ventre J (1985b) Cortical control and oculomotor functions. II. Vestibuloocular reflex and visual vestibular interaction. Behav Brain Res 17:227–234

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood CC, Spear PD, Braun JJ (1974) Effects of sequential lesions of suprasylvian gyri and visual cortex on pattern discrimination in the cat. Brain Res 66:443–466

    Google Scholar 

  • Yin TCT, Medjbeur S, El Barbary A, Felsten G (1986) Visual response properties of cells in the middle and lateral suprasylvian cortices on the behaving cat. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 27:155

    Google Scholar 

  • Yin TCT, Greenwood M (1992a) Visual response properties of neurons in the middle and lateral suprasylvian cortices of the behaving cat. Exp Brain Res 88:1–14

    Google Scholar 

  • Yin TCT, Greenwood M (1992b) Visuomotor interactions in responses of neurons in the middle and lateral suprasylvian cortices of the behaving cat. Exp Brain Res 88:15–32

    Google Scholar 

  • Zumbroich TJ, Grünau M von, Poulin C, Blakemore C (1986) Differences of visual field representation in the medial and lateral banks of the suprasylvian cortex (PMLS, PLLS) of the cat. Exp Brain Res 64:77–93

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Krüger, K., Kiefer, W., Groh, A. et al. The role of the lateral suprasylvian visual cortex of the cat in object-background interactions: Permanent deficits following lesions. Exp Brain Res 97, 40–60 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00228816

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00228816

Key words

Navigation