Skip to main content
Log in

The role of automatic negative thoughts in the development of dysphoric mood: An analogue experiment

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Beck's cognitive theory of depression asserts that active, depressogenic schemas produce a thinking pattern characterized by negative thoughts concerning the self, the world, and the future. Factor II of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ-30) is assumed to be consistent with two (views of self and future) of the three components of this negative cognitive triad. It was hypothesized that nondepressed subjects exhibiting a high frequency of automatic negative thoughts according to their scores on Factor II would be more sensitive to Velten's Mood Induction Procedure (VMIP) than subjects exhibiting a low frequency of such thoughts. The results indicated that high ATQ-30 Factor II scores predicted significantly more depression on the Depression Adjective Checklist (DACL) and lower psychomotor speed as measured on the Digit Symbol Test. High ATQ-30 Factor II scores had no effect on the Minnesota Clerical Test and the Arithmetic Problems. The results support the view that a high frequency of automatic negative thoughts toward the self and the future in nondepressed subjects may indicate a vulnerability to depression at the moment of testing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87 49–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrew, D. M., & Paterson, D. (1959).Manual for the Minnesota Clerical Test (rev. ed.). New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1976).Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Kovacs, M., & Weissman, A. (1975). Hopelessness and suicidal behavior: An overview.Journal of the American Medical Association, 324 1146–1149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979).Cognitive therapy of depression: A treatment manual. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buchwald, A. M., Strack, S., & Coyne, J. C. (1981). Demand characteristics and the Velten mood induction procedure.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49 478–479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bumberry, W., Oliver, J. M., & McClure, J. N. (1978). Validation of the Beck Depression Inventory in a university population using psychiatric estimate as a criterion.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46 150–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. M. (1983). On the induction of depressed mood in the laboratory: Evaluation and comparison of the Velten and musical procedures.Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 5 27–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crandell, C. J., & Chambless, D. L. (1986). The validation of an inventory for measuring depressive thoughts: The Crandell Cognitions Inventory.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24 403–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dobson, K. S., & Breiter, H. J. (1983). Cognitive assessment of depression: Reliability and validity of three measures.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92 107–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, A. M., & Williams, J. M. G. (1982). Mood-induction research: Its implications for clinical depression.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 20 373–382.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollon, S. D., & Kendall, P. C. (1980). Cognitive self-statements in depression: Development of an automatic thoughts questionnaire.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4 383–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollon, S. D., Kendall, P. C., & Lumry, A. (1986). Specificity of depressotypic cognitions in clinical depression.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95 52–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewinsohn, P. M., Steinmetz, J. L., Larson, D. W., & Franklin, J. (1981). Depression-related cognitions: Antecedent or consequence?Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90 213–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lubin, B. (1965). Adjective checklists for measurement of depression.Archives of General Psychiatry, 12 57–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lubin, B., Hornstra, R. K., & Love, A. (1974). Course of depressive mood in a psychiatric population upon application for service at 3- and 12-months reinterviews.Psychological Reports, 34 424–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, R. (1974). Relationship between mood and digit-symbol performance in hospitalized patients with functional psychiatric disorders.Psychological Medicine, 4 454–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, M. (1985). Neuroticism as predisposition toward depression: A cognitive mechanism.Personality and Individual differences, 6 353–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, W. R. (1975). Psychological deficit in depression.Psychological Bulletin, 82 238–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polivy, J. (1981). On the induction of emotion in the laboratory: Discrete moods or multiple affect states?Journal of Personality and Social psychology, 41 803–817.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polivy, J., & Doyle, C. (1980). Laboratory induction of mood states through the reading of self-referent mood statements: Affective changes or demand characteristics?Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89 286–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyszczynski, T., Holt, K., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Depression, self-focused attention, and expectancies for positive and negative future life events for self and others.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52 994–1001.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rholes, W. S., Riskind, J. H., & Neville, B. (1985). The relationship of cognitions and hopelessness to depression and anxiety.Social Cognition, 3 36–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., & Rholes, W. S. (1984). Cognitive accessibility and the capacity of cognitions to predict future depression: A theoretical note.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, T. C., & Götestam, K. G. (1988).Cognitive vulnerability to depression: A longitudinal study. Unpublished manuscript, University of Trondheim.

  • Stiles, T. C., & Götestam, K. G. (1988). The role of cognitive vulnerability factors in the development of depression. In C. Perris, I. M. Blackburn, & H. Perris (Eds.),Cognitive psychotherapy. Theory and practice. (pp. 120–139). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D. (1983). Negative thinking in depression: Cause, effect, or reciprocal relationship.Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 5 3–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Velten, E. (1968). A laboratory task for inductions of mood states.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 6 473–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1955).Wechsler adult intelligence scale: Manual. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weissman, A. N., & Beck, A. T. (1978).Development and validation of the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale. Paper presented at the Annual meeting of the Association for Advancement of Behavioral Therapy, Chicago.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Stiles, T.C., Götestam, K.G. The role of automatic negative thoughts in the development of dysphoric mood: An analogue experiment. Cogn Ther Res 13, 161–170 (1989). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01173270

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01173270

Key words

Navigation