Skip to main content
Log in

Merging different motor patterns: Coordination between rhythmical and discrete single-joint movements

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Subjects made fast, discrete elbow flexion movements while simultaneously producing rhythmical oscillations about initial and final visual targets embedded on a horizontal surface. Based on kinematic and electromyographic (EMG) analysis, we found that the discrete movement could start at any phase of the cyclical movement. The most likely onset time occurred when the first agonist burst started at the same moment as a rhythmical burst would have appeared. This resulted in a smooth conjugation between discrete and rhythmical movements. The initiation of the discrete movement was associated with the resetting of the phase of the rhythmical movements. Thus, the time characteristics of the two motor tasks were interdependent. A subset of trials with a uniform distribution of discrete movement onset phases could be selected in most subjects and was averaged to eliminate the cyclical component from the combined movement. Mean kinematic and EMG traces up until the peak velocity were practically identical to those of the discrete movement made alone. The averaging procedure was ineffective in eliminating the rhythmical component following the discrete movement because of the resetting of the phase of oscillation. Using the same procedure it has been shown that initiating the rhythmical movement at the same time as beginning the discrete movement did not affect the initial part of discrete movement. The whole discrete movement was not affected when subjects simultaneously terminated the ongoing rhythmical movements. Our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that although the rhythmical movement constrains the onset time of discrete movement, the latter, once initiated, proceeds independently of the ongoing rhythmical movement. We also subtracted the discrete component from the combined movement to see how the former affected the rhythmical movement. The residual pattern showed that the rhythmical movements rapidly attenuated when the discrete movement started and then apparently resumed after the peak velocity of the discrete movement. The results corroborate the hypothesis that the control signals underlying the two motor tasks cannot be applied simultaneously, since they may be associated with conflicting stability requirements. Instead, these control signals may be generated sequentially, but the resulting kinematic responses may outlast them and be superposed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ackroyd MH (1973) Digital filters. Butterworth, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Adamovich SV, Feldman AG (1984) Model of the central regulation of the parameters of motor trajectories. Biophysics 29:338–342

    Google Scholar 

  • Adamovich SV, Burlachkova NI, Feldman AG (1984) Wave nature of the central process of formation of the trajectories of change in the joint angle in man. Biophysics 29:130–134

    Google Scholar 

  • Adamovich SV, Levin MF, Feldman AG (1992) Association between rhythmical and discrete components of movements. Soc Neurosci Abstr 18:1054

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersson O, Grillner S (1981) Peripheral control of the cat's step cycle. I. Phase dependent effects of ramp-movements of the hip during “fictive locomotion”. Acta Physiol Scand 113:89–101

    Google Scholar 

  • Asatryan DS, Feldman AG (1965) Functional tuning of the nervous system with control of movement or maintenance of a steady posture. 1. Mechanographic analysis of the work of the joint on execution of a postural task. Biophysics 10:925–935

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkinblit MB, Feldman AG, Fukson OI (1986) Adaptability of innate motor patterns and motor control mechanisms. Behav Brain Sci 9:585–638

    Google Scholar 

  • Bizzi E, Accornero N, Chapple W, Hogan N (1984) Posture control and trajectory formation during arm movements. J Neurosci 4:2738–2744

    Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan JJ, Kelso JAS (1993) Posturally induced transitions in rhythmic multijoint limb movements. Exp Brain Res 94:131–142

    Google Scholar 

  • Deluca CJ, Mambrito B (1987) Voluntary control of motor units in human antagonist muscles: coactivation and reciprocal activation. J Neurophysiol 58:525–42

    Google Scholar 

  • Dietz V, Quintern J, Berger W (1984) Corrective reactions to stumbling in man: functional significance of spinal and transcortical reflexes. Neurosci Lett 44:131–135

    Google Scholar 

  • Drew T, Rossignol S (1987) A kinematic and electromyographic study of cutaneous reflexes evoked from the forelimb of unrestrained walking cats. J Neurophysiol 57:1160–1184

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman AG (1966) Functional tuning of the nervous system with control of movement or maintenance of a steady posture. III. Mechanographic analysis of execution by man of the simplest motor task. Biophysics 11:667–675

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman AG (1979) Central and reflex mechanisms of motor control (in Russian). Nauka, Moscow

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman AG (1980a) Superposition of motor programs. I. Rhythmic forearm movements in man. Neuroscience 5:81–90

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman AG (1980b) Superposition of motor programs. II. Rapid flexion of forearm in man. Neuroscience 5:91–95

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman AG (1993) The coactivation command for antagonist muscles involving Ib interneurons in mammalian motor control systems: an electrophysiologically testable model. Neurosci Lett 155:167–170

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman AG, Levin MF (1993) Control variables and related concepts in motor control. Concepts Neurosci 4:25–51

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman AG, Adamovich SV, Ostry DJ, Flanagan JR (1990) The origin of electromyograms — explanations based on the equilibrium point hypothesis. In: Winters JM, Woo SL-Y (eds) Multiple muscle systems. Biomechanics and movement organization. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 195–221

    Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan JR, Ostry DJ, Feldman AG (1993) Control of trajectory modifications in target-directed reaching. J Mot Behav 25:140–152

    Google Scholar 

  • Flash T (1990) The organization of human arm trajectory control. In: Winters JM, Woo SL-Y (eds) Multiple muscle systems. Biomechanics and movement organization. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 282–301

    Google Scholar 

  • Forssberg H (1979) Stumbling corrective reactions: a phase-dependent compensatory reaction during locomotion. J Neurophysiol 42:936–953

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Georgopoulos AP, Kalaska JF, Massey JT (1981) Spatial trajectories and reaction times of aimed movements: effects of practice, uncertainty, and change in target location. J Neurophysiol 461:725–743

    Google Scholar 

  • Giszter SF, McIntyre J, Bizzi E (1989) Kinematic strategies and sensori-motor transformations in the wiping movements of frogs. J Neurophysiol 62:750–767

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman D, Kelso JAS (1983) Exploring the functional significance of physiological tremor: a biospectroscopic approach. Exp Brain Res 49:419–431

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb GL, Corcos DM, Agarwal GC (1989) Strategies for the control of voluntary movements with one mechanical degree of freedom. Behav Brain Sci 12:189–210

    Google Scholar 

  • Grillner S (1985) Central pattern generators for locomotion, with special reference to vertebrates. Annu Rev Neurosci 8:233–261

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasan Z (1986) Optimized movement trajectories and joint stiffness in unperturbed, inertially loaded movements. Biol Cybern 53:372–382

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogan N (1984) An organizing principle for a class of voluntary movements. J Neurosci 4:2745–2754

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey DR, Reed DJ (1983) Separate cortical systems for control of joint movement and joint stiffness: reciprocal activation and coactivation of antagonist muscles. Adv Neurol 39:347–72

    Google Scholar 

  • Katayama T, Kohase H, Nakamura Y (1993) Resetting of cortically induced rhythmical jaw movements by stimulation of the cerebellar interpositus nucleus in the guinea pig. Brain Res 617:143–146

    Google Scholar 

  • Keder-Stepanova IA (1967) The characteristics of respiratory neurons at different levels of the central nervous system. In: Models of structural-functional organization of some biological systems. MIT, Boston pp 95–105

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso JAS, Southard DL, Goodman D (1979) On the nature of human interlimb coordination. Science 203:1029–1031

    Google Scholar 

  • Lacquaniti F (1992) Automatic control of limb movement and posture. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2:807–814

    Google Scholar 

  • Lan N, Crago PE (1992) Equilibrium-point hypothesis, minimum effort strategy and the triphasic muscle activation pattern. Behav Brain Sci 15:769

    Google Scholar 

  • Latash ML (1993) Control of human movement. Human Kinetics, Champaign, IL

    Google Scholar 

  • Latash ML, Gottlieb GL (1991) Reconstruction of joint compliant characteristics during fast and slow movements. Neuroscience 43:697–712

    Google Scholar 

  • Latash ML, Gutman SR, Gottlieb GL (1991) Relativistic effects in single-joint voluntary movements. Biol Cybern 65:401–406

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin MF, Feldman AG, Milner TE, Lamarre Y (1992) Reciprocal and coactivation commands for fast wrist movements. Exp Brain Res 89:669–677

    Google Scholar 

  • Milner TE, Ijaz MM (1990) The effect of accuracy constraints on three-dimensional movement kinematics. Neuroscience 35:365–374

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagasaki H (1992) Asymmetrical trajectory formation in cyclic forearm movements in man. Exp Brain Res 87:653–661

    Google Scholar 

  • Parush A, Ostry DJ (1993) Lower pharyngeal wall movement in speech. J Acoust Soc Am 94:715–722

    Google Scholar 

  • Sergio EL, Ostry DJ (1993) Three-dimensional kinematic analysis of frog hindlimb movement in reflex wiping. Exp Brain Res 94:53–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Staude G, Wolf W, Wierzbicka MM, Dengler R (1992) Tremor as a factor in delayed reaction time. Soc Neurosci Abstr 18:862

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein RB (1982) What muscle variable(s) does the nervous system control in limb movements? Behav Brain Sci 5:535–577

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein PSG, Johnstone KM (1986) Motor patterns during rostal scratching and forward swimming in the turtle. Soc Neurosci Abstr 12:790

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein PSG, Camp AW, Robertson GA, Mortin LI (1986) Blends of rostral and caudal scratch reflex motor patterns elicited by stimulation of two sites in the spinal turtle. J Neurosci 6:2259–2266

    Google Scholar 

  • St-Onge N, Qi H, Feldman AG (1993) The patterns of control signals underlying elbow joint movements in humans. Neurosci Lett 164:171–174

    Google Scholar 

  • Viviani P, Terzuolo C (1982) Trajectory determines movement dynamics. Neuroscience 7:431–437

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Adamovich, S.V., Levin, M.F. & Feldman, A.G. Merging different motor patterns: Coordination between rhythmical and discrete single-joint movements. Exp Brain Res 99, 325–337 (1994). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00239599

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00239599

Key words

Navigation