Skip to main content
Log in

The har strains of rats: Origins and characteristics

  • Genetic Considerations
  • Published:
Behavior Genetics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Origins of the principal lines ofRattus norvegicus used in behavior genetic research are identified. Broad descriptions are provided for 15 of the Har strains and substrains of well-established inbred lines and behaviorally selected stocks. Marker genes are identified for coat and major histocompatibility complex (MHC) loci. Detailed characteristics are summarized for 12 of these lines using a comprehensive behavioral test battery composed of a variety of measures of expressive behavior, learning, and physiological characteristics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bassett, G. C. (1914). Habit formation in a strain of albino rats of less than normal brain weight.Behav. Monogr. 2:4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bignami, G. (1965). Selection for high rates and low rates of conditioning in the rat.Anim. Behav. 13:221–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • blizard, D. A. (1981). The Maudsley Reactive and Nonreactive strains: A North American perspective.Behav. Genet. 11:469–489.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadhurst, P. L. (1957). Determinants of emotionality in the rat. I. Situational factors.Br. J. Psychol. 48:1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadhurst, P. L. (1975). The Maudsley Reactive and Nonreactive strains of rats: A survey.Behav. Genet. 5:299–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brody, S. (1964).Bioenergetics and Growth, Hafner, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brush, F. R., Froehlick, J. C., and Sakellaris, P. C. (1979). Genetic selection for avoidance behavior in the rat.Behav. Genet. 9:309–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farris, E. J., and Griffith, J. Q. (1962).The Rat in Laboratory Investigation, Haber, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Festing, M., and Staats, J. (1973). Standardized nomenclature for inbred strains of rats.Transplantation 16:221–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. S. (1951). The genetics of behavior. In Stevens, S. S. (ed.),Handbook of Experimental Psychology, Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1966). Genetic specificity in maze performance.Psychonom. Sci. 6:103–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1968). Genetic-environmental interaction in “intelligence.” I. Biometric genetic analysis of maze performance ofRattus norvegicus.Dev. Psychobiol. 1:211–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1971a). Strain differences among rats initiating exploration of differing environments.Psychonom. Sci. 23:348–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1971b). Strain differences in rotating wheel activity of the rat.Psychonom. Sci. 23:363–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1972). Strain differences in open field behavior of the rat.Psychonom. Sci. 27:51–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1979a). Strain differences in activity of the rat in a shuttle stabilimeter.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 13:149–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1979b). Strain differences in activity of the rat using a home cage stabilimeter.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 13:151–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1979c). Strain differences in free operant leverpress levels in the rat.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 13:153–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1979d). Strain differences in light-contingent barpress behavior of the rat.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 13:155–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1979e). Strain differences in neophilia in the rat.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 14:424–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1979f). Strain differences in open-field behavior of the rat. II.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 13:85–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1979g). Strain differences in passive avoidance conditioning in the rat.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 13:157–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1979h). Strain differences in runway learning in the rat.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 13:159–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1979i). Strain differences in shuttle avoidance conditioning in the rat.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 13:161–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1979j). Strain differences in simple operant barpress acquisition to an auditory stimulus by rats.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 13:163–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M., and Gill, T. J., III (1981). Histocompatibility typing of Har lines (submitted for publication).

  • Harrington, G. M., and Hellwig, L. R. (1979a). Strain differences in basal metabolism of behaviorally defined rats.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 13:165–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M., and Hellwig, L. R. (1979b). Strain differences in organ weights of behaviorally defined rats.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 13:167–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O., and Williams, K. (1946). A method of rating animal intelligence.J. Gen. Psychol. 34:59–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarvik, M. E., and Kopp, R. (1967). An improved one-trial passive avoidance learning situation.Psychol. Rep. 21:221–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lonowski, D. J., Levitt, R. A., and Larson, S. D. (1975). Mouse killing and carrying by Maudsley and Long-Evans strain rats.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 6:629–631.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maier, N. R. F., and Glaser, N. M. (1940). Studies of abnormal behavior in the rat. V. The inheritance of the “neurotic pattern.”J. Comp. Psychol. 30:413–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masur, J., and Benedito, M. A. C. (1974). Genetic selection of winner and loser rats in a competitive situation.Nature 249:284.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDougall, W. (1938). Fourth report on a Lamarckian experiment.Br. J. Psychol. 20:321–345, 365–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundl, W. J. (1966). Activity of small animals measured with accelerometer.Med. Biol. Eng. 4:209–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Philipeaux, J. M. (1856). Note sur l'exterpation des capsules surrenales chez les rats albinos (Mus rattus).C. R. Acad. Sci. Paris 43:904–906.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabinovitch, M. S., and Rosvold, H. E. (1951). A closed-field intelligence test for rats.Can. J. Psychol. 5:122–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowland, G. L., and Woods, P. J. (1961). Performance of the Tryon bright and dull strains under two conditions in a multiple T-maze.Can. J. Psychol. 15:20–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruch, T. C., and Patton, H. D. (1965).Physiology and Biophysics, 19th ed., Saunders, Philadelphia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satinder, K. P. (1972). Behavior-genetic-dependent self-selection of alcohol in rats.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 80:422–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satinder, K. P. (1976a). Reactions of selectively bred strains of rats to a cat.Anim. Learn. Behav. 4:172–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satinder, K. P. (1976b). Sensory responsiveness and avoidance learning in rats.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 90:946–957.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, L. V. (1949). The organization of hereditary maze-brightness and maze-dullness.Genet. Psychol. Monogr. 39:279–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Small, W. S. (1899). Notes on the psychic development of the young white rat.Am. J. Psychol. 11:80–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Small, W. S. (1900). An experimental study of the mental processes of the rat.Am. J. Psychol. 11:133–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. (1924). Inheritance of maze learning ability in rats.J. Comp. Psychol. 4:1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tryon, R. C. (1940). Genetic differences in maze-learning ability in rats.39th Yearb. Natl. Soc. Study Educ. 1:111–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, L. L. (1980). Differences in food, water and food-deprivation water intake in 16 strains of rats.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 94:775–781.

    Google Scholar 

  • Womack, J. E., and Cramer, D. V. (1980). Peptidase-3 (Pep-3), Dipeptidase variant in the rat homologous to mouse Pep-3 (Dip-1) and human PEP-C.Biochem. Genet. 18:1019–1026.

    Google Scholar 

  • Worden, A. N., and Lane-Petter, W. (1957).The UFAW Handbook on Care and Management of Laboratory Animals, UFAW, London.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Harrington, G.M. The har strains of rats: Origins and characteristics. Behav Genet 11, 445–468 (1981). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01070003

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01070003

Key words

Navigation