Skip to main content
Log in

Effect of 6-hydroxydopamine-induced lesions of the dorsal noradrenergic bundle on steady-state operant behaviour

  • Original Investigations
  • Published:
Psychopharmacology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The possible role of the dorsal noradrenergic bundle (DNAB) in the maintenance of operant behaviour by positive reinforcement was examined using a quantitative behavioural paradigm based on Herrnstein's (1970) equation which defines a hyperbolic relationship between steady-state response rate and reinforcement frequency in variable-interval schedules. Twelve rats received bilateral injections of 6-hydroxydopamine (4 μg/2 μl) into the DNAB; ten rats received sham injections. The rats were trained to steady state in a series of six variable-interval schedules of sucrose reinforcement affording reinforcement frequencies of 8–350 reinforcers per hour. Herrnstein's equation was fitted to the data obtained from each rat and to the averaged data obtained from the two groups. The values of both R max (the parameter of the equation expressing the theoretical maximum response rate) and K H (the parameter expressing the reinforcement frequency needed to maintain the half-maximal response rate) were significantly higher in the DNAB-lesioned group than in the sham-lesioned group. At the end of the behavioural experiment the rats were sacrificed for determination of catecholamine levels in the brain by high-performance liquid chromatography. The levels of noradrenaline in the neocortex and hippocampus of the DNAB-lesioned rats were approximately 10% of those of the sham-lesioned rats. The results indicate that destruction of the DNAB reduced the “value” of the reinforcer without impairing the animals' capacity to respond.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bradshaw CM (1977) Suppression of response rates in variable-interval schedules by a concurrent schedule of reinforcement. Br J Psychol 68:473–480

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E (1987) Quantitative analysis of human operant behaviour. In: Davey GCL (ed) Human operant conditioning and behaviour modification. Wiley, Chichester (in press)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E, Bevan P (1976) Behavior of humans in variable-interval schedules of reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav 26:135–141

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E, Bevan P (1978) Relationship between response rate and reinforcement frequency in variable-interval schedules: the effect of the concentration of sucrose reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav 29:447–452

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw CM, Ruddle HV, Szabadi E (1981a) Relationship between response rate and reinforcement frequency in variable-interval schedules: II. Effect of the volume of sucrose reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav 35:263–269

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw CM, Ruddle HV, Szabadi E (1981b) Relationship between response rate and reinforcement frequency in variable-interval schedules: III. Effect of d-amphetamine. J Exp Anal Behav 36:28–39

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E, Ruddle HV (1983a) Herrnstein's equation: Effect of response-force requirement on performance in variable-interval schedules. Behav Anal Lett 3:93–100

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E, Ruddle HV (1983b) Herrnstein's equation: Effect of deprivation level on performance in variable-interval schedules. Behav Anal Lett 3:267–273

    Google Scholar 

  • Catania AC, Reynolds GS (1968) A quantitative analysis of the responding maintained by interval schedules of reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav 11:327–383

    Google Scholar 

  • Christensen AV, Arnt J, Hyttel J, Larsen J-J, Svendsen O (1984a) Pharmacological effects of a specific dopamine D-1 antagonist SCH 23390 in comparison with neuroleptics. Life Sci 34:1529–1540

    Google Scholar 

  • Christensen AV, Arnt J, Hyttel J, Svendsen O (1984b) Behavioral correlates to the dopamine D-1 and D-2 antagonists. Pol J Pharmacol Pharm 36:249–264

    Google Scholar 

  • Clavier RM, Fibiger HC, Phillips AC (1976) Evidence that self-stimulation of the region of the locus coeruleus in rats does not depend upon noradrenergic projections to telencephalon. Brain Res 113:71–81

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper BR, Cott JM, Breese GR (1974) Effects of catecholamine-depleting drugs and amphetamine on self-stimulation of brain following various 6-hydroxydopamine treatments. Psychopharmacologia 37:235–248

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper BR, Konkol RJ, Breese GR (1978) Effects of catecholamine-depleting drugs and d-amphetamine on self-stimulation of the substantia nigra and locus coeruleus. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 204:592–605

    Google Scholar 

  • Crow TJ (1973) Catecholamine-containing neurones and electrical self-stimulation: 2. A theoretical interpretation and some psychiatric implications. Psychol Med 3:66–73

    Google Scholar 

  • Crow TJ, Deakin JFW (1985) The neurochemistry of behaviour, the pharmacology of centrally acting drugs and the mechanism of the disturbance in psychiatric disease. In: Shepherd M, Zangwill OL (eds) Handbook of Psychiatry, vol 5. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Crow TJ, Wendlandt S (1976) Impaired acquisition of a passive avoidance response after lesions induced in the locus coeruleus by 6-OH-dopamine. Nature 259:42–44

    Google Scholar 

  • Deakin JFW, Crow TJ (1986) Monoamines, rewards and punishments: the anatomy and physiology of the affective disorders. In: Deakin JFW (ed) The biology of depression. Gaskell, London, pp 1–25

    Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers PA (1977) Choice in concurrent schedules and a quantitative formulation of the law of effect. In: Honig WK, Staddon JER (eds) Handbook of operant behavior Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey, pp 233–287

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray JA (1982) The neuropsychology of anxiety. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray JA, Owen S, Davis N, Tsaltas E (1983) Psychological and physiological relations between anxiety and impulsivity, In: Zuckerman M (ed) Biological bases of sensation seeking, impulsivity, and anxiety. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp 181–227

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton AL, Stellar JR, Hart EB (1985) Reward, performance, and the response strength method in self-stimulating rats: validation and neuroleptics. Physiol Behav 35:897–904

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein RJ (1970) On the law of effect. J Exp Anal Behav 13:243–266

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein RJ (1974) Formal properties of the matching law. J Exp Anal Behav 21:159–164

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyman GM, Monaghan MM (1987) The effect of changes in the response requirement and deprivation on the parameters of the matching law equation: new data and review. J Exp Psychol (in press)

  • Jonsson G, Hallman H, Ponzio F, Ross SB (1981) DSP4 (N-2-chloroethyl-N-ethyl-2-bromobenzylamine)- a useful denervation tool for central and peripheral noradrenaline neurones. Eur J Pharmacol 72:173–188

    Google Scholar 

  • Klisans-Fuenmayor D, Harston CT, Kostrzewa RM (1986) Alterations in noradrenergic innervation of the brain following dorsal bundle lesions in neonatal rats. Brain Res Bull 16:47–54

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis D (1960) Quantitative methods in psychology. Springer, New York Berlin Heidelberg

    Google Scholar 

  • McDowell JJ (1981) Wilkinson's method of estimating the parameters of Herrnstein's equation. J Exp Anal Behav 35:413–414

    Google Scholar 

  • McNaughton N, Mason ST (1980) The neuropsychology and neuropharmacology of the dorsal ascending noradrenergic bundle — a review. Prog Neurobiol 14:157–219

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason ST (1984) Catecholamines and behaviour. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason ST, Iversen SD (1977) Effects of selective forebrain noradrenaline loss on behavioural inhibition. J Comp Physiol Psychol 91:165–173

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mason ST, Iversen SD (1979) Theories of the dorsal bundle extinction effect. Brain Res Rev 1:107–137

    Google Scholar 

  • Morley MJ, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E (1985) The effect of d-amphetamine on operant behaviour maintained under variable-interval schedules of reinforcement. Psychopharmacology 87:207–211

    Google Scholar 

  • Morley MJ, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E (1987) DSP4 alters the effect of d-amphetamine on variable-interval performance: analysis in terms of Herrnstein's equation. Psychopharmacology 92:247–253

    Google Scholar 

  • Owen S, Boarder M, Gray JA, Fillenz M (1982) Acquisition and extinction of continuously and partially reinforced running in rats with lesions of the dorsal noradrenergic bundle. Behav Brain Res 5:11–41

    Google Scholar 

  • Pisa M, Fibiger HC (1983) Intact selective attention in rats with lesions of the dorsal noradrenergic bundle. Behav Neurosci 97:519–529

    Google Scholar 

  • Price MT, Murray GN, Fibiger HC (1977) Schedule dependent changes in operant responding after lesions of the dorsal tegmental noradrenergic projection. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 6:11–15

    Google Scholar 

  • Robbins TW, Everitt BJ (1982) Functional studies of central catecholamines. Int Rev Neurobiol 23:303–364

    Google Scholar 

  • Robbins TW, Everitt BJ, Cole BJ, Archer T, Mohammed A (1985) Functional hypotheses of the coeruleocortical noradrenergic projection: A review of recent experimentation and theory. Physiol Psychol 13:127–150

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruddle HV, Morley MJ, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E (1984) The effect of pentobarbitone on variable-interval performance: analysis in terms of Herrnstein's equation. Psychopharmacology 84:520–525

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein L (1962) Effects and interactions of imipramine, chlorpromazine, reserpine and amphetamine on self-stimulation: Possible neurophysiological basis of depression. In: Wortis J (ed) Recent advances in biological psychiatry, vol 4 Plenum, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein L (1983) The chemistry of positive reinforcement. In: Zuckermann M (ed) Biological bases of sensation seeking, impulsivity, and anxiety. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp 151–180

    Google Scholar 

  • Tombaugh TN, Pappas BA, Roberts DCS, Vickers GJ, Szostak C (1983) Failure to replicate the dorsal bundle extinction effect: telencephalic norepinephrine depeletion does not reliably increase resistance to extinction but does augment gustatory neophobia. Brain Res 261:231–242

    Google Scholar 

  • Ungerstedt U (1971) Stereotaxic mapping of the monoamine pathways in the rat brain. Acta Physiol Scand [Suppl 362] 82:1–48

    Google Scholar 

  • Waterhouse RD, Woodward DJ (1980) Interaction of norepinephrine with cerebrocortical activity evoked by stimulation of somatosensory afferent pathways in the rat. Exp Neurol 67:11–34

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson GN (1961) Statistical estimations in enzyme kinetics. Biochem J 80:324–332

    Google Scholar 

  • Worms P, Broekkamp CLE, Lloyd KG (1983) Behavioral effects of neuroleptics. In: Coyle JT, Enna SJ (eds) Neuroleptics: neurochemical, behavioral and clinical perspectives. Raven, New York, pp 93–117

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Morley, M.J., Bradshaw, C.M. & Szabadi, E. Effect of 6-hydroxydopamine-induced lesions of the dorsal noradrenergic bundle on steady-state operant behaviour. Psychopharmacology 93, 520–525 (1987). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00207246

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00207246

Key words

Navigation