Library

feed icon rss

Your email was sent successfully. Check your inbox.

An error occurred while sending the email. Please try again.

Proceed reservation?

Export
  • 1
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Springer
    Experimental brain research 44 (1981), S. 317-324 
    ISSN: 1432-1106
    Keywords: Cats ; Visual development ; Eye rotation ; Amblyopia ; Suppression
    Source: Springer Online Journal Archives 1860-2000
    Topics: Medicine
    Notes: Summary Visual pattern and brightness discriminations were tested in cats which had undergone surgical cyclodeviations of one or both eyes. Half of the pattern discriminations were learned with both eyes open and performance was tested with each eye separately; the other half were learned monocularly with the normal eye (in cats with monocular rotations, MR) or with the less amblyopic eye (in cats with binocular rotations, BR) and then the rotated (or amblyopic) eye was tested alone. No deficits were found in brightness discriminations. With monocularly learned pattern discriminations all but one cat showed positive savings when tested with the rotated eye. However, on binocularly learned discriminations half of the animals performed poorly with the rotated eye and required extensive retraining; they showed negative savings when compared to original learning. These animals seem to be suppressing the strabismic eye during binocular vision. For MR cats, there was a positive relationship between visual acuity and percent savings after binocular learning. The relationship between amblyopia and suppression suggests a common cause.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Library Location Call Number Volume/Issue/Year Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 2
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Springer
    Experimental brain research 113 (1997), S. 291-302 
    ISSN: 1432-1106
    Keywords: Orienting response ; Visual fixation ; Attentive fixation ; Optic tectum
    Source: Springer Online Journal Archives 1860-2000
    Topics: Medicine
    Notes: Abstract Neurons in the rostral superior colliculus (SC) of alert cats exhibit quasi-sustained discharge patterns related to the fixation of visual targets. Because some SC neurons also respond to auditory stimuli, we investigated whether there is a population of neurons in the rostral SC which is active in relation to fixation of both auditory and visual targets. We identified cells which were active with visual fixation and which continued to discharge if the fixation stimulus was briefly extinguished. The population of neurons exhibited similar discharge characteristics when the fixation stimulus was auditory. Few neurons were significantly more active during fixation of visual targets than during fixation of auditory targets. Most fixation neurons showed a diminished discharge rate during spontaneous (self-generated) saccadic eye movements away from a visual fixation stimulus, regardless of the direction of the saccade. this diminished discharge rate (or pause) typically began, on average, 12.2 ms before saccade onset and the duration of the pause was Ionger than the duration of the saccade. These observations are consistent with the hypothesis that increased discharge of these neurons is related to active fixation and that reductions in their activity are important for the generation of saccades. However, the lack of a precise relationship between pause duration and saccade duration implies that these neurons would be unlikely to project directly to the saccadic burst generator. The mean interval from the beginning of the pauses of fixation neurons to be beginning of the saccades away from fixation targets is also shorter than has been found in brainstem omnipause neurons. By analogy with the concept of a receptive field, agaze position error field depicts the range of gaze position error for which a cell is active. Although fixation neurons appear to encode the magnitude and direction of the error between visual targets and the visual axis, visual error fields at the end of fixating eye movements were significantly larger than those at stimulus onset. For auditory stimuli, this difference was not significant. These observations are compatible with a number of recent experiments indicating that neural signals of eye position are damped or delayed with respect to current eye position.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Library Location Call Number Volume/Issue/Year Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 3
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Springer
    Experimental brain research 115 (1997), S. 25-34 
    ISSN: 1432-1106
    Keywords: Key words Reaction time ; Saccadic latency ; Saccadic eye movement ; Ocular motor system ; Human
    Source: Springer Online Journal Archives 1860-2000
    Topics: Medicine
    Notes: Abstract  Recent neurophysiological studies of the saccadic ocular motor system have lent support to the hypothesis that this system uses a motor error signal in retinotopic coordinates to direct saccades to both visual and auditory targets. With visual targets, the coordinates of the sensory and motor error signals will be identical unless the eyes move between the time of target presentation and the time of saccade onset. However, targets from other modalities must undergo different sensory-motor transformations to access the same motor error map. Because auditory targets are initially localized in head-centered coordinates, analyzing the metrics of saccades from different starting positions allows a determination of whether the coordinates of the motor signals are those of the sensory system. We studied six human subjects who made saccades to visual or auditory targets from a central fixation point or from one at 10° to the right or left of the midline of the head. Although the latencies of saccades to visual targets increased as stimulus eccentricity increased, the latencies of saccades to auditory targets decreased as stimulus eccentricity increased. The longest auditory latencies were for the smallest values of motor error (the difference between target position and fixation eye position) or desired saccade size, regardless of the position of the auditory target relative to the head or the amplitude of the executed saccade. Similarly, differences in initial eye position did not affect the accuracy of saccades of the same desired size. When saccadic error was plotted as a function of motor error, the curves obtained at the different fixation positions overlapped completely. Thus, saccadic programs in the central nervous system compensated for eye position regardless of the modality of the saccade target, supporting the hypothesis that the saccadic ocular motor system uses motor error signals to direct saccades to auditory targets.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Library Location Call Number Volume/Issue/Year Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 4
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Springer
    Experimental brain research 122 (1998), S. 247-252 
    ISSN: 1432-1106
    Keywords: Key words Multisensory integration ; Saccade ; Reaction time ; Gaze shifts ; Human
    Source: Springer Online Journal Archives 1860-2000
    Topics: Medicine
    Notes: Abstract  Information from the auditory and visual systems converges in the nervous system with physiological and behavioral consequences. Most of our knowledge about the rules governing such convergence has been obtained in experiments where the strength or the timing of the individual auditory and visual stimuli has been varied. Relatively little attention has been paid to the spatial relationship between different modalities of stimuli in multisensory experiments. We studied saccadic reaction times of human subjects to bimodal auditory and visual stimulus presentations under two conditions: first, with the targets spatially coincident and, second, with various degrees of spatial separation or disparity. In the first experiment, we found that the saccadic reaction times were consistently shorter than would be predicted by independent processing of information about the visual and auditory targets. These results suggest convergence of multimodal information at one or more loci within the nervous system. In the second experiment, we found that saccadic latency gradually increased as spatial distance between the auditory and visual targets increased. Evidence for neural summation was found over a wide range of spatial disparities. These results suggest that multisensory information can be integrated and have significant influences on behavior over a surprisingly large range of spatial disparity.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Library Location Call Number Volume/Issue/Year Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 5
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Springer
    Experimental brain research 61 (1986), S. 447-450 
    ISSN: 1432-1106
    Keywords: Superior colliculus ; Single units ; Eye movements ; Cat
    Source: Springer Online Journal Archives 1860-2000
    Topics: Medicine
    Notes: Summary Single unit activity was studied in the intermediate and deep layers of the superior colliculus in two trained cats. Eye movements were recorded with a magnetic search coil, the head being fixed. Discharge rates which varied as a function of eye position were consistently observed in 7 of 67 (about 10%) of the sample of eye movement-related units. These units showed similar changes in firing rate as a function of eye position in total darkness and during task related fixation of visual targets and thus appear to convey an “eye position” signal. Their activity may originate either from proprioception or from corollary discharge.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Library Location Call Number Volume/Issue/Year Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 6
    ISSN: 1432-1106
    Keywords: Cats ; Eye rotation ; Visuomotor adaptation ; Pattern discrimination ; Interocular transfer
    Source: Springer Online Journal Archives 1860-2000
    Topics: Medicine
    Notes: Summary Sixteen cats, each of which had one or both eyes rotated at the time of natural eye opening (group K), were tested for visuomotor behavior and for learning and interocular transfer of two-choice visual discriminations. Their behavior was compared to that of two cats given monocular rotations in adulthood (group A) and to two normal controls (group N). These animals were all reared in the same colony. All cats, including those with monocular rotations up to 180 ° and those with binocular rotations up to 80 ° in each eye, showed good visuomotor behavior when using the rotated eye (i.e., with the normal eye covered). Both the group K and group A animals showed comparable visuomotor adaptation. All animals except those with monocular rotations of 180 ° were able to learn several oriented pattern discriminations and showed considerable though incomplete interocular transfer of such information. The three animals with 180 ° rotations were able to learn brightness, but not pattern discriminations. Seven further animals with large rotations were used for histological studies of the retina and primary visual pathways. Areas of reduced ganglion cell density were not observed in whole mounts of the retinae, nor were regions of reduced transport of 3H-proline from the retina to the lateral geniculate nuclei or superior colliculi detectable from autoradiographs.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Library Location Call Number Volume/Issue/Year Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 7
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Springer
    Experimental brain research 41 (1980), S. 61-74 
    ISSN: 1432-1106
    Keywords: Cats ; Eye rotation ; Visual perimetry ; Visual fields ; Visuomotor adaptation
    Source: Springer Online Journal Archives 1860-2000
    Topics: Medicine
    Notes: Summary Visual fields of ten cats which had one or both eyes rotated at 8 days of age were measured by two forms of perimetry and compared to visual fields of two normal cats and of four cats with monocular rotations at 16 days, 3 months or 6 months of age. All animals showed excellent localization of visual stimuli and responded to the actual location of stimuli in space rather than to the retinal locus normally associated with that location. In cats with monocular rotations, the field of the normal eye was always normal, extending from 90 ° ipsilateral to 30 ° contralateral. Cats with rotations of one eye at 3 or 6 months of age had essentially normal fields in the rotated eye as well, while cats with surgery at 8 or 16 days had restricted horizontal fields. They responded only to stimuli in the ipsilateral hemifield; they were blind in the contralateral hemifield. Their superior and inferior visual fields were normal. The field deficits related consistently to visual field coordinates and not to the angle or direction of rotation. In cats with binocular rotations the visual field of at least one eye extended across the midline. Thus, the extent of the field depended upon sensorimotor experiences of the cat both before and after surgery. It is argued that these monocular field deficits have a central origin, not a retinal one. When tested with both eyes open, seven of 14 experimental animals did not respond throughout the visual field seen by each eye alone. The total visual field with both eyes open was less than the sum of the two monocular fields; greatest losses were most pronounced in the extreme periphery of the field ipsilateral to the rotated eye. Since changes in eye position (e.g., convergence during bincocular viewing) were not observed, it is suggested that the binocular losses indicate suppression of the deviated eye which has a central origin. All animals were tested for visual following, visually-triggered extension (placing), and visually-guided reaching. Cats which had been routinely encouraged to use the rotated eye(s) by occlusion of the other eye showed skilful performance within a few weeks after surgery as previously reported by Peck and Crewther (1975), Mitchell et al. (1976) and others. In contrast, two cats reared with both eyes open after unilateral rotation in infancy were profoundly handicapped, as previously reported by Yinon (1975, 1976).
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Library Location Call Number Volume/Issue/Year Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 8
    ISSN: 1432-1106
    Keywords: Visual receptive field ; Spatial coding ; Absolute position ; Gaze control ; Nonspecific thalamus
    Source: Springer Online Journal Archives 1860-2000
    Topics: Medicine
    Notes: Summary Visual receptive field properties of neurons in the region of the thalamic internal medullary lamina were studied in alert cats while they fixated in various directions. In slightly more than 50% of the cells, the responsiveness of the cells was found to depend on the location of the stimulus with respect to the head-body axis (stimulus absolute position). A cell could ignore a stimulus outside its absolute field even if it was well placed within its receptive field. Three types of neurons were distinguished. Neurons with small central receptive fields were tonically activated when the animal fixated the stimulus in one half of the screen (usually contralateral). The firing rate of these cells was related to the stimulus absolute position measured along a preferred axis. Similarly, neurons with large receptive fields fired as a function of stimulus absolute position but stimulus fixation was not required. Neurons with eccentric fields responded to stimuli located in a target area defined in head-body coordinates. Such cells gave presaccadic bursts with eye movements terminating in the target area. The conclusion proposed is that neurons exist which code visual spatial information in a non-retinal frame of reference. This coding takes place at the time of stimulus presentation. Its role may be seen in the initiation of visually guided movements.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Library Location Call Number Volume/Issue/Year Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 9
    ISSN: 0550-3213
    Source: Elsevier Journal Backfiles on ScienceDirect 1907 - 2002
    Topics: Physics
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Library Location Call Number Volume/Issue/Year Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 10
    ISSN: 0550-3213
    Source: Elsevier Journal Backfiles on ScienceDirect 1907 - 2002
    Topics: Physics
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Library Location Call Number Volume/Issue/Year Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
Close ⊗
This website uses cookies and the analysis tool Matomo. More information can be found here...